A9VG电玩部落论坛

 找回密码
 注册
搜索
楼主: alex_to

关于乔是否杀无辜、在艰难时期干过猎人的行当替kazi1229发表公开致歉信

[复制链接]

精华
0
帖子
4589
威望
0 点
积分
4705 点
种子
5 点
注册时间
2010-10-15
最后登录
2020-12-13
 楼主| 发表于 2013-9-5 21:56  ·  云南 | 显示全部楼层
air0001 发表于 2013-9-5 21:10
作者给出了生存多样性但同时也没有回避末世生存可能要靠损人利己牺牲他人这种手段活下去的必然性。反而直 ...

自行查阅我与metalgearliquid 提出的论据吧

精华
0
帖子
2641
威望
0 点
积分
2690 点
种子
5 点
注册时间
2009-2-27
最后登录
2016-9-2
发表于 2013-9-6 23:02  ·  江苏 | 显示全部楼层
kazi1229 发表于 2013-9-5 22:41
这个问题每个人都有自己的看法,我只是基于这个游戏本身来讨论这个角色,个人喜好我不干涉,就像很多人喜 ...

最近看的几个很好的片子
没有过去的男人
天国的车站
我要复仇

精华
1
帖子
10157
威望
1 点
积分
10692 点
种子
487 点
注册时间
2005-10-27
最后登录
2024-11-14
发表于 2013-9-7 12:53  ·  天津 | 显示全部楼层
air0001 发表于 2013-9-6 23:02
最近看的几个很好的片子
没有过去的男人
天国的车站

我要复仇有日本的和韩国的,你说的是哪个?

精华
1
帖子
10157
威望
1 点
积分
10692 点
种子
487 点
注册时间
2005-10-27
最后登录
2024-11-14
发表于 2013-9-7 13:49  ·  天津 | 显示全部楼层
alex_to 发表于 2013-9-6 01:10
“你是个根本不看剧情完全靠想象的人啊,这就是你最牛的地方,活在自己阿Q的世界,然后再自己的世界你什么 ...

粗略地看了看,不知所云,说有人扫大街有人富二代就成为仇富了?那换个说法,有人天生智力高达200是天才,有人天生***,他们能选择的范围一样?估计你又该说我歧视***了,从来不敢正面回答问题,先把别人的话引到错误的范畴再去否定,你这种招数在辩论里太低级了,耍无赖的级别,你不喜欢思考无所谓,幼稚是你的特色,请继续保持,呵呵

精华
0
帖子
4589
威望
0 点
积分
4705 点
种子
5 点
注册时间
2010-10-15
最后登录
2020-12-13
 楼主| 发表于 2013-9-7 18:01  ·  云南 | 显示全部楼层
kazi1229 发表于 2013-9-7 13:49
粗略地看了看,不知所云,说有人扫大街有人富二代就成为仇富了?那换个说法,有人天生智力高达200是天才, ...

“说有人扫大街有人富二代就成为仇富了?”——那你到底、究竟想说明什么问题呢(没有白手起家?乔是富二代?)?
“有人天生智力高达200是天才,有人天生***,他们能选择的范围一样?”——他们能选择的范围可能一样吗?这就是生存手段高低的根本原因!——智商低的人犯罪就能够受得到法律的保护、道德的原谅吗(除非是精神病患者——但不等同于智商低!你报以同情、温情的猎人没有手?没有脚?还是没有头脑?)?
“从来不敢正面回答问题,先把别人的话引到错误的范畴再去否定”——你提过哪怕有那么一丁点价值的问题我不但正面回答并且剖析出你的懵懂认知、匪夷所思的加以严厉苛责让你汗颜,所以以你的眼界自然会认为被我引导,你有眼界、远见卓识会被引导吗?
“耍无赖的级别,你不喜欢思考无所谓,幼稚是你的特色”——骗得了别人虽然不可取但至少说明比别人的智商高;骗自己、骗一千遍,自己就活在谎言中、臆境中、可悲中……


           ——能遇到我这样的良师益友是你今生的福气!你听得进去小则令人刮目相看、大则为你消灾减祸!

精华
0
帖子
653
威望
0 点
积分
993 点
种子
608 点
注册时间
2006-2-4
最后登录
2024-10-22
发表于 2013-9-7 18:01  ·  香港 | 显示全部楼层
本帖最后由 wtpmsing 于 2013-9-7 18:22 编辑
alex_to 发表于 2013-9-6 01:10
“你是个根本不看剧情完全靠想象的人啊,这就是你最牛的地方,活在自己阿Q的世界,然后再自己的世界你什么 ...


在动物界里经常发生动物吃掉兒女的事情,如你曾養兔子,倉鼠等你更知道.只要她觉得不能養育那么多的数量便会吃掉其中一头,你看熊猫只要生双胞胎便棄掉其中一头養育,

而你说人生來的性善和性恶.这等不用讨论了!!!!

已有科学家做實驗了

人的天性是多方面的,有善亦有惡,視乎對人類進化是否有利。



相关的研究報告耶鲁大学的the baby lab刊登
http://www.yale.edu/infantlab/Welcome.html

最切實的研究是問問初生嬰兒,看看他們對善惡的傾向。但初生嬰兒是白紙一張.
但耶魯大學的心理學家發展新研究方法,探討嬰兒的潛在意向。
他們在大學設立了嬰兒實驗室 Baby Lab,嘗試找出道德的起源。

在CBS的60 minutes有專提報導 Youtube

http://www.***.com/watch?v=UF90BzQZPfQ

中文簡要    英文是详细访问

六十分鐘時事特輯對 Baby Lab 造了一個專訪,介紹研究成果。

研究的方法是讓三至六個月大的嬰兒看一些玩偶表演,然後讓他們選擇其中一個玩偶。嬰兒們雖然仍未能伸手取玩偶,但研究員會從他們的眼神,決定他們多望那一個玩偶,然後引申他們對那一種玩偶的行為有好感。

兩個玩偶爭奪玩具;A详细访问搶走B的玩具;嬰兒們望著B的次數在統計上較多,研究說嬰兒已會分善惡,對弱者有愛心,所以性本善。

另一個實驗是再加一個C玩偶,對A作出懲罰。
嬰兒們望著C的次數在統計上較多,研究說嬰兒已有仇恨感,對C懲罰他人的行為有好感。

研究結論是嬰兒在出生時對道德已有概念,不過善和惡的傾向都同時存在。

详细访问
    Born good? Babies help unlock the origins of morality

The following script is from "The Baby Lab" which aired on Nov. 18, 2012, and was rebroadcast on July 28, 2013. Lesley Stahl is the correspondent. Shari Finkelstein, producer.

It's a question people have asked for as long as there have been people: are human beings inherently good? Are we born with a sense of morality or do we arrive blank slates, waiting for the world to teach us right from wrong? Or could it be worse? Do we start out nasty, selfish devils, who need our parents, teachers, and religions to whip us into shape?

As we first reported last fall, the only way to know for sure, is to ask a baby. But until recently, it's been hard to persuade them to open up and share their secrets. Enter the baby lab.

This is the creature at the center of the greatest philosophical, moral, and religious debates about the nature of man: the human baby. They don't do much, can't talk, can't write, can't expound at length about their moral philosophies. But does that mean they don't have one? The philosopher Rousseau considered babies "perfect idiots...Knowing nothing," and Yale psychologist Karen Wynn, director of the Infant Cognition Center here, the baby lab, says for most of its history, her field agreed.

Lesley Stahl: Didn't we just think that these creatures at three months and even six months were basically just little blobs?

Karen Wynn: Oh, sure. I mean, if you look at them, they--

Lesley Stahl: Yeah.

Karen Wynn: They kinda look like little, I mean, cute little blobs. But they can't do all the things that an older child can. They can't even do the things that a dog or a pigeon or a rat can.

No pulling levers for treats or running mazes for these study subjects. But they can watch puppet shows. And Wynn is part of a new wave of researchers who have discovered seemingly simple ways to probe what's really going on in those adorable little heads. We watched as Wynn and her team asked a question that 20 years ago might have gotten her laughed out of her field. Does Wesley here, at the ripe old age of 5 months, know the difference between right and wrong?

Wesley watches as the puppet in the center struggles to open up a box with a toy inside. The puppy in the yellow shirt comes over and lends a hand. Then the scene repeats itself, but this time the puppy in the blue shirt comes and slams the box shut. Nice behavior...mean behavior...at least to our eyes. But is that how a 5-month-old sees it, and does he have a preference?

Annie: Wesley, do you remember these guys from the show?

To find out, a researcher who doesn't know which puppet was nice and which was mean, offers Wesley a choice.

Annie: Who do you like?

He can't answer, but he can reach... (reaches for nice puppet)

Annie: That one?

Wesley chose the good guy and he wasn't alone.

More than three fourths of the babies tested reached for the nice puppet. Wynn tried it out on even younger babies, 3 month olds, who can't control their arms enough to reach. But they can vote with their eyes, since research has shown that even very young babies look longer at things they like. Daisy here looked at the mean puppet for 5 seconds; then switched to the nice one for 33.

Karen Wynn: Babies, even at three months, looked towards the nice character and looked hardly at all, much, much, much shorter times, towards the unhelpful character.

Lesley Stahl: So basically as young as three months old, we human beings show a preference for nice people over mean people.

Karen Wynn: Study after study after study, the results are always consistently babies feeling positively towards helpful individuals in the world. And disapproving, disliking, maybe condemning individuals who are antisocial towards others.

Lesley Stahl: It's astonishing.

Wynn and her team first published their findings about baby morality in the journal "Nature" in 2007, and they've continued to publish follow-up studies in other peer-reviewed journals ever since -- for instance on this experiment.

They showed babies like James here a puppet behaving badly -- instead of rolling the ball back to the puppet in the middle, this green-shirted bunny keeps the other puppet's ball, and runs away.

Then James is shown a second show -- this time the bunny who he just saw steal the ball, tries to open up the box to get the toy. Will James still prefer the puppet who helps out? Or will he now prefer the one who slams the box shut?

[Annie: Who do you like? That one.]

He chose the one who slammed it shut, as did 81 percent of babies tested. The study's conclusion: babies seem to view the ball thief "as deserving punishment."

Lesley Stahl: So do you think that babies, therefore, are born with an innate sense of justice?

Karen Wynn: At a very elemental level, I think so.
Paul Bloom: We think we see here the foundations for morality.

Paul Bloom is also a professor of psychology at Yale, with his own lab. He's collaborated with Wynn on many of her baby studies, and he also happens to be her husband.

Paul Bloom: I feel we're making discoveries. I feel like we're-- we're discovering that what seems to be one way really isn't. What seems to be an ignorant and unknowing baby is actually a creature with this alarming sophistication, this subtle knowledge.

And he says discovering this in babies who can't walk, talk, or even crawl yet, suggests it has to come built in.

Lesley Stahl: So, remember B.F. Skinner, who said that we had to teach our children everything through conditioning. So, does this just wipe him off the map?

Paul Bloom: What we're finding in the baby lab, is that there's more to it than that -- that there's a universal moral core that all humans share. The seeds of our understanding of justice, our understanding of right and wrong, are part of our biological nature.

Wait a minute, if babies are born with a basic sense of right and wrong, a universal moral core, where does all the evil in the world come from? Is that all learned? Well maybe not. Take a look at this new series of discoveries in the Yale baby lab...

[Annie: Would you like a snack?]

In offering babies this seemingly small, innocuous choice -- graham crackers or Cheerios -- Wynn is probing something big: the origins of bias. The tendency to prefer others who are similar to ourselves.

Karen Wynn: Adults will like others who share even really absolutely trivial similarities with them.

So will Nate, who chose Cheerios over graham crackers, prefer this orange cat, who also likes Cheerios -- over the grey cat who likes graham crackers instead?

Apparently so. But if babies have positive feelings for the similar puppet, do they actually have negative feelings for the one who's different? To find out, Wynn showed babies the grey cat -- the one who liked the opposite food, struggling to open up the box to get a toy. Will Gregory here want to see the graham cracker eater treated well? Or does he want him treated badly?

[Annie: Which one do you like? That one.]

Gregory seemed to want the different puppet treated badly.

Lesley Stahl: That is amazing. So he went with his bias in a way.

And so did Nate and 87 percent of the other babies tested. From this Wynn concludes that infants prefer those "who harm... others" who are unlike them.

Paul Bloom: What could be more arbitrary than whether you like graham crackers or Cheerios?

Lesley Stahl: Nothing.

Paul Bloom: Nothing. But it matters. It matters to the young baby. We are predisposed to break the world up into different human groups based on the most subtle and seemingly irrelevant cues, and that, to some extent, is the dark side of morality.

Lesley Stahl: We want the other to be punished?

Karen Wynn: In our studies, babies seem as if they do want the other to be punished.

Lesley Stahl: We used to think that we're taught to hate. I think there was a song like that. This is suggesting that we're not taught to hate, we're born to hate.

Karen Wynn: I think, we are built to, you know, at the drop of a hat, create us and them.

Paul Bloom: And that's why we're not that moral. We have an initial moral sense that is in some ways very impressive, and in some ways, really depressing -- that we see some of the worst biases in adults reflected in the minds and in the behaviors of young babies.

But Bloom says understanding our earliest instincts can help...

Paul Bloom: If you want to eradicate racism, for instance, you really are going to want to know to what extent are babies little bigots, to what extent is racism a natural part of humanity.

Lesley Stahl: Sounds to me like the experiment show they are little bigots.

Paul Bloom: I think to some extent, a bias to favor the self, where the self could be people who look like me, people who act like me, people who have the same taste as me, is a very strong human bias. It's what one would expect from a creature like us who evolved from natural selection, but it has terrible consequences.

He says it makes sense that evolution would predispose us to be wary of "the other" for survival, so we need society and parental nurturing to intervene. He showed us one last series of experiments being done in his lab -- not with babies, but with older children of different ages. The kids get to decide how many tokens they'll get, versus how many will go to another child they're told will come in later. They're told the tokens can be traded in for prizes.

[Mark: So you can say green, and if you say green, then you get this one and the other girl doesn't get any; or you can say blue, and if you say blue, then you get these two, and the other girl gets these two. So green or--

The youngest kids in the study will routinely choose to get fewer prizes for themselves just to get more than the other kid --

[Ainsley: I'll pick green.]

-- in some cases, a lot more.

Paul Bloom: The youngest children in the studies are obsessed with social comparison.

[Mark: So you get these seven. She doesn't get any.

Kendall: Yay!]

Paul Bloom: They don't care about fairness. What they want is they want relatively more.

But a funny thing happens as kids get older. Around age 8, they start choosing the equal, fair option more and more. And by 9 or 10, we saw kids doing something really crazy --

[Abby: Green.]

-- deliberately giving the other kid more.

Mark: Green or blue?

Maeve: Green.

They become generous. Chalk one up to society.

Lesley Stahl: They've already been educated?

Paul Bloom: They've been educated, they've been in***urated, they have their heads stuffed full of the virtues that we might want to have their heads stuffed with.

So we can learn to temper some of those nasty tendencies we're wired for -- the selfishness, the bias -- but he says the instinct is still there.

Paul Bloom: When we have these findings with the kids, the kids who choose this and not this, the kids in the baby studies who favor the one who is similar to them, the same taste and everything-- none of this goes away. I think as adults we can always see these and kind of nod.

Lesley Stahl: Yeah. It's still in us. We're fighting it.

Paul Bloom: And the truth is, when we're under pressure, when life is diffi***, we regress to our younger selves and all of this elaborate stuff we have on top disappears.

But of course adversity can bring out the best in us too -- heroism, selfless sacrifice for strangers -- all of which may have its roots right here.

Paul Bloom: Great kindness, great altruism, a magnificent sense of impartial justice, have their seeds in the baby's mind. Both aspects of us, the good and the bad are the product I think of biological evolution.

And so it seems we're left where we all began: with a mix of altruism, selfishness, justice, bigotry, kindness. A lot more than any of us expected to discover in a blob.

Lesley Stahl: Well, I end my conversation with you with far more respect for babies. Who knew?





精华
0
帖子
653
威望
0 点
积分
993 点
种子
608 点
注册时间
2006-2-4
最后登录
2024-10-22
发表于 2013-9-7 18:04  ·  香港 | 显示全部楼层
kazi1229 发表于 2013-9-5 23:02
你是个根本不看剧情完全靠想象的人啊,这就是你最牛的地方,活在自己阿Q的世界,然后再自己的世界你什么想 ...

kaz   我支持你   

精华
0
帖子
4589
威望
0 点
积分
4705 点
种子
5 点
注册时间
2010-10-15
最后登录
2020-12-13
 楼主| 发表于 2013-9-7 18:14  ·  云南 | 显示全部楼层
wtpmsing 发表于 2013-9-7 17:31
“虎毒食子”老虎吃掉三幼虎

虎毒不食子——作为一句成语是用来形容“人比禽兽更残忍”,不是作为科学依据——所以你百度一番究竟想说明什么问题???

精华
0
帖子
4589
威望
0 点
积分
4705 点
种子
5 点
注册时间
2010-10-15
最后登录
2020-12-13
 楼主| 发表于 2013-9-7 18:18  ·  云南 | 显示全部楼层
本帖最后由 alex_to 于 2013-9-7 18:24 编辑
wtpmsing 发表于 2013-9-7 18:04
kaz   我支持你


你的支持有POWER吗?我可以说你甚至是在害他吗?

有新论据关于乔杀了无辜、困难时期干过猎人的行当吗?——欣喜若狂期待中……

精华
0
帖子
653
威望
0 点
积分
993 点
种子
608 点
注册时间
2006-2-4
最后登录
2024-10-22
发表于 2013-9-7 18:27  ·  香港 | 显示全部楼层
alex_to 发表于 2013-9-7 18:18
你的支持有POWER吗?

有新论据关于乔杀了无辜、困难时期干过猎人的行当吗?——欣喜若狂期待中……

the anw is "both side "不否认也不承认
那你有什么新理根据證名他没有干过呢?
您需要登录后才可以回帖 登录 | 注册

本版积分规则

Archiver|手机版|A9VG电玩部落 川公网安备 51019002005286号

GMT+8, 2024-11-15 05:56 , Processed in 0.184512 second(s), 13 queries , Redis On.

Powered by Discuz! X3.4

Copyright © 2001-2020, Tencent Cloud.

返回顶部